(par 5.1.1) Hunter-Gatherers or Foragers

A VPN is an essential component of IT security, whether you’re just starting a business or are already up and running. Most business interactions and transactions happen online and VPN
http://hraf.yale.edu/resources/faculty/explaining-human-culture/hunter-gatherers-foragers-2/

Carol R. Ember

In the quest to explain human culture, anthropologists have paid a great deal of attention to recent hunter-gatherer, or forager, societies. A major reason for this focus has been the widely held belief that knowledge of hunter-gatherer societies could open a window into understanding early human cultures. After all, it is argued, for the vast stretch of human history, people lived by foraging for wild plants and animals. Indeed, not until about 10 thousand years ago did societies in Southwest Asia (the famous Fertile Crescent) begin to cultivate and domesticate plants and animals. Food production took over to such an extent that, in the past few hundred years, only an estimated 5 million people have subsisted by foraging.

hunter gatherers 3Rock paintings, like this undated example of human figures and antelopes by the San in Namibia, southern Africa, provide rare graphic evidence of earlier hunter-gatherer life. Rock art exists throughout the world but is difficult to date. The oldest dated example, found in Australia, is believed to be 28,000 years old.

What can we infer about our distant ancestors by looking at a few well- known hunter-gatherer societies of recent times? To draw reliable inferences, we would need to believe that pockets of human society could exist unchanged over tens of thousands of years—that hunter- gatherers did not learn from experience, innovate, or adapt to changes in their natural and social environments. Even a cursory look at the ethnographic record, however, reveals that many foraging cultures have changed substantially over time. Moreover, recent hunter- gatherer cultures share some traits but are also quite different from one another.

How can we draw better inferences about the past? Cross-cultural researchers ask how and why hunter-gatherer societies vary. By understanding what conditions predict variation and also using the paleoanthropological record to make educated guesses about past conditions in a particular place, anthropologists may have a better chance of inferring what hunter-gatherers of the past was like (Hitchcock and Beisele, 2000, p. 5; C. R. Ember, 1978; Marlowe, 2005)

Because cultures change through time, we cannot simply project ethnographic data from the present to the past.

Below we summarize the cross-cultural literature in the last half century on hunter-gatherers. We generally restrict the discussion to statistically supported hypotheses based on samples of 10 or more cultures. We also discuss what is not yet known and questions that invite further research.

What We Have Learned

We know about hunter-gatherers of recent times from anthropologists who have lived and worked with hunting and gathering groups. Some of the best recently known cases are the Mbuti of the Ituri Forest (central Africa), the San of the Kalahari Desert (southern Africa) and the Copper Inuit of the Arctic (North America). These hunter-gatherers live in environments that are not conducive to agriculture.

What are hunter-gatherers of recent times generally like?

Based on the ethnographic data and cross-cultural comparisons, it is widely accepted that recent hunter-gatherer societies

  • are fully or semi-nomadic.
  • live in small communities.
  • have low population densities.
  • do not have specialized political officials.
  • have little wealth differentiation.
  • are economically specialized only by age and gender .
  • usually divide labor by gender, with women gathering wild plants and men fishing and almost always doing the hunting.

Some cross-cultural findings are less widely discussed:

  • Compared to food producers, hunter-gatherers are less likely to stress obedience and responsibility in child training. On the other hand, hunter-gatherer cultures that emphasize hunting are more likely to stress achievement in children. (Barry, Child, and Bacon, 1959; Hendrix, 1985)
  • Compared to food producers, hunter-gatherers show more warmth and affection toward their children. (Rohner, 1975, pp. 97-105)
  • The songs of hunter-gatherers are less wordy and characterized by more nonwords, repetition, and relaxed enunciation (Lomax 1968, pp. 117-28)
  • In contrast to food producers, hunter-gatherers are less prone to resource unpredictability, famines, and food shortages. (Textor 1967; Ember and Ember, 1997:10)

Are hunter-gatherers more peaceful than food producers?

Some cross-cultural findings contradict each other, inviting further investigation:

  • it is widely agreed that, compared to food producers, hunter- gatherers fight less (Ember & Ember, 1997). But are hunter- gatherers typically peaceful? Different researchers have arrived at different answers to this question. For example, Ember (1978) reported that most hunter-gatherers engaged in warfare at least every two years. Another study found that warfare was rare or absent among most hunter-gatherers (Lenski & Lenski, 1978; reported in Nolan, 2003).

Hunter-gatherer cultures differ from food-producing cultures in childrearing practices and vocalization. Food-producing cultures are more vulnerable to famines and food shortages.

hunter gatherers 4A Himba Boy Cooks Lunch

How we define terms will affect the sample and determine the outcome of a cross-cultural study. When asking if hunter-gatherers are typically peaceful, for example, researchers will get different results depending upon what they mean by peaceful, how they define hunter- gatherers, and whether they have excluded societies forced to stop fighting by colonial powers or national governments.

Most researchers contrast war and peace. If the researcher views peace as the absence of war, then the answer to whether hunter-gatherers are more peaceful than food producers depends on the definition of war. Anthropologists agree that war in smaller-scale societies needs to be defined differently from war in nation-states that have armed forces and large numbers of casualties. Also, within-community or purely individual acts of violence are nearly always distinguished from warfare. However, there is controversy about what to call different types of socially organized violence between communities. For example, Fry (2006: 88, 172-174) does not consider feuding between communities warfare.

How and why do hunter-gatherers vary?

Hunter-gatherers vary in many ways, but cross-cultural research has focused on variations in types of food -getting, contributions to the diet by gender, the degree of nomadism, the frequency of external and internal warfare, and marital residence.

  • The closer to the equator, the higher the effective temperature, or the more plant biomass, the more hunter-gatherers depend upon gathering rather than hunting or fishing. (Lee, 1968, pp. 42-43; Kelly, 1995, p. 70; Binford, 1990, pp. 132)
  • The lower the effective temperature, the more hunter- gatherers rely on fishing. (Binford, 1990, p. 134)
  • Males contribute more to the diet the lower the effective temperature or the higher the latitude. (Kelly, 1995, p. 262; Marlowe, 2005, p. 56)
  • In higher quality environments (with more plant growth), men are more likely to share gathering with women. Greater division of labor by gender occurs in lower quality environments. (Marlowe, 2007)
  • Fully nomadic lifestyles are more likely as the growing season lengthens. (Binford, 1990, p. 131)
  • Among hunter-gatherers, in contrast to other kinds of societies, division of labor predicts marital residence. The more a foraging society depends upon gathering, the more likely the society is to be matrilocal. The more dependence upon fishing, the more likely a society is to be patrilocal. Degree of dependence on hunting does not predict marital residence. (Ember, 1975)
  • Patrilocal hunter-gatherers do not have more warfare than those that are matrilocal. Among foragers, as in other societies, patrilocal residence is predicted by internal (within society) warfare or a high male contribution to subsistence; matrilocality is predicted by a combination of purely external warfare and a high female contribution to subsistence. (Ember, 1975)
  • Bilocal residence, rather than unilocal residence, is predicted by community size under 50, high rainfall variability, and recent drastic population loss. (Ember, 1975)
  • Hunter-gatherers with higher population densities have more warfare than those with low population densities. Similarly, more complex hunter-gatherer societies have more warfare than simpler hunter-gatherers. (Nolan, 2003, p. 26; Kelly, 2000,pp. 51–52); Fry, 2006, p. 106)

hunter gatherers 5Tlingit women and children cleaning fish on beach, southeastern Alaska, ca.1907.

  • Hunter-gatherers with a high dependence on fishing are more likely to have internal warfare than external warfare. (Ember, 1975)
  • In New Guinea, foragers with a high dependence on fishing tend to have higher population density and large settlements. Some of the foragers in New Guinea with a high dependence on fishing have densities of 40 or more people/square km and settlements of over 1000 people. (Roscoe, 2006)

What We Do Not Know

hunter gatherers 6San Hunters in the Kalahari Desert, Namibia.

  • Why do some foraging societies share more than others? Is meat consistently shared more than plants? Does sharing differ by gender?
  • Why should division of labor predict residence amongst hunter- gatherers, but not among food-producing cultures? (See Ember, 1975)
  • Do foragers with a high dependence on fishing tend to have higher population density and large settlements, as is the case in New Guinea? (See Roscoe, 2006)
  • How different are foragers with a little agriculture from those who lack agriculture?
  • Are foragers with horses more like pastoralists than foragers lacking horses?
  • Recently, discussion of the differences between complex and simple hunter-gatherers has increased. (See, for example,Fitzhugh, 2003; Sassaman, 2004). Complex hunter-gatherers generally have considerable inequality and more political hierarchy.
  • What other differences are there between complex and simple hunter-gatherers?
  • What implications do such differences have for the emergence of complex foragers?

hunter gatherers 7Tlingit Chief Charles Jones Shakes, pictured at home in Wrangell, Alaska, with an array of his possessions, ca. 1907. The Tlingit, a culture dependent on fishing, exemplify the hierarchical structure of complex hunter-gatherer societies

Exercises Using eHRAF World Cultures

The module should be cited as:

Carol R. Ember. 2014. “Hunter-Gatherers (Foragers)” in C. R. Ember, Explaining Human Culture. Human Relations Area Files, http://hraf.yale.edu/hunter-gatherers-foragers, accessed [give date].

References Cited

Barry III, Herbert, Irvin L. Child & Margaret K. Bacon. 1959. Relation of child training to subsistence economy. American Anthropologist, 61, 51-63.

Binford, Lewis R. 1990. Mobility, housing, and environment: A comparative study. Journal of Anthropological Research, 46, 119-152.

Ember, Carol R. 1975. Residential variation among hunter-gatherers. Cross-Cultural Research, 10, 199-227.

Ember, Carol R. 1978. Myths about hunter-gatherers. Ethnology, 17, 439-448.

Ember, Carol R. & Melvin Ember. 1997. Violence in the ethnographic record: Results of cross-cultural research on war and aggression. In Debra L. Martin & David W. Frayer (Eds.), Troubled times: Violence and warfare in the past (pp. 1-20). New York, NY: Routledge.

Fitzhugh, Ben. 2003. The evolution of complex hunter-gatherers: interdisciplinary contributions to archaeology. Springer.

Fry, Douglas P. 2006. The human potential for peace: An anthropological challenge to assumptions about war and violence. New York: Oxford University Press.

Hendrix, Lewellyn. 1985. Economy and child training reexamined. Ethos, 13, 246-261.

Hitchcock, Robert K. & Megan Beisele. 2000. “Introduction.” In Peter P. Schweitzer, Megan Biesele & Robert K. Hitchcock (Eds.), Hunters and gatherers in the modern world: Conflict, resistance, and self-determinations (pp. 1-27), New York: Berghahn Books.

Kelly, Robert L. 1995. The foraging spectrum. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.

Kelly, Raymond C. 2000. Warless societies and the origin of war. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Press.

Lee, Richard B. & Irven DeVore (Eds.). 1968. Man the hunter. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company.

Lomax, Alan (Ed.). 1968. Folk song style and culture. Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science Publication No. 88.

Marlowe Frank W. 2001. Male contribution to the diet and female reproductive success among foragers. Current Anthropology, 42, 755-760.

–2004. Marital residence among foragers. Current Anthropology, 45, 277-284.

–2005. Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 14, 56-67.

–2007. Hunting and Gathering: The human sexual division of foraging labor. Cross-Cultural Research, 41, 170-195.

Murdock, George P. & Caterina Provost. 1973. Factors in the division of labor by sex: A cross-cultural analysis. Ethnology, 12, 203-225.

Nolan, Patrick D. 2003. Toward an ecological-evolutionary theory of the incidence of warfare in preindustrial societies. Sociological Theory, 21, 18-30.

Rohner, Ronald. 1975. They love me, they love me not: A worldwide study of the effects of parental acceptance and rejection. New Haven, CT: HRAF Press.

Roscoe, Paul. 2006. Fish, game, and the foundations of complexity in forager society: The evidence from new guinea. Cross-Cultural Research, 40, 29-46.

Sassaman, Kenneth. 2004. Complex hunter-gatherers in evolution and history: A North American perspective. Journal of Anthropological Research, 12, 227-280.

Service, Elman R. 1979. The hunters. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Textor, Robert B. 1967. A cross-cultural summary. New Haven, CT: HRAF Press.

Additional Cross-Cultural Studies of Hunter-Gatherers

Halperin, Rhonda H. 1980. Ecology and mode of production: Seasonal variation and the division of labor by sex among hunter-gatherers. Journal of Anthropological Research, 36, 379-399.

Korotayev, Andrey V. & Alexander A. Kazankov. 2003. Factors of sexual freedom among foragers in cross-cultural perspective. Cross-Cultural Research, 37, 29-61.

Lozoff, Betsy and Gary Brittenham. 1979. Infant care: Cache or carry. The Journal of Pediatrics, 95, 478-483.

Marlowe, Frank W. 2003. The mating system of foragers in the standard cross-cultural sample. Cross-Cultural Research, 37, 282-306.

 
krtadmin

krtadmin

Leave a Replay

Share

Recent Posts

Follow Us

Supporting Videos